Monday, September 7, 2020
Happy Labor Day 2020
Friday, July 31, 2020
The Memory of Places Long Gone
I had a dream last night about my old inpatient psychiatric unit. I was moving up and down the hallways like I used to do looking for people to interview. It always seemed like I had to track people down in order to talk with them. In my dream, I was in and out of familiar rooms and talking with familiar staff. I have known some of the staff people for over 20 years. The place was congested, noisy, and malodorous. There were the associated tensions and anxiety. Everything was in the exact spatial order that I remember it being in - including all of the disarray in my old office. I eventually woke up from that dream somewhat anxious and decided that I was done sleeping.
As I drank a first cup of coffee, I thought about the
various meanings in the dream and developed three or four different threads. I
am used to analyzing my own dreams and the dreams of others so I had all my
theories finalized by my 2nd cup of coffee. At that point I started
to think about the visuospatial memory necessary to create that dream. Dreams
are certainly fascinating if you just pay attention to the content and the
emotional tone but to be they are more fascinating when you consider how much
computational power it takes to create a Technicolor dream of your real life
experience. I have asked neuroscientists to speculate about this for years now and
nobody has come up with any ideas. I
came up with a few of my own based on very simplistic extrapolations of the
bandwidth of single neurons and simplistic bundles of neurons. One of the
problems is that bandwidth and information calculations are valid only for
typical electronic devices. Bandwidth and biological systems has been talked
about for 20 years but there are no straightforward or consistent
calculations.
A first approximation might be a two-dimensional
photograph. Everyone these days is used to seeing photographs and an estimate
of how much information is in that picture. But when you are walking through a
building, you are essentially walking through an infinite number of
two-dimensional photographs with multiple soundtracks and additional sources of
information. Even if a measure of the information it takes to portray that data
in real time and space was possible the next question is - what happens when it
is represented in the brain?
I pulled up Facebook and noticed that someone had posted a
photo of my old high school. As I looked at the front of the school I located
all the windows where my classes were held. I was in that building for the last
six years of school because in our town it contained grades seven through 12.
It took me a few minutes to look at the windows and think about significant
events that occurred in those classes. When I think about those years, there is
a lot of confusion and anxiety. I did not have a very positive experience in
middle school or high school. I was one of the most obedient kids in the
history of the world and yet I managed to have contentious relationships with
teachers. Back in those days if you were on the wrong side of the teachers – you
were basically on the wrong side. Parents
didn’t question teacher authority and I knew better than to bring that information back
home. But just like my dream I was focused on the structure this building. I
still remember the creaky wooden floors and stairwells. I remember the bad
acoustics and echoes. I remember where my locker was on the first floor. I
remember what the place smelled like. I remembered how to go to the ground
floor and take the tunnel to a connected building where I had some regular
classes but also mechanical drawing and technical graphics. In other words, I
still had the memory of the three dimensional space - long after that space had
ceased to exist. I made a short list of other structures where that was also
true including my old inpatient unit. That building was demolished about three
years ago.
If you think about it visuospatial memory is a fascinating
area of study. It takes more than remembering a list of words or calculation or
how to play a musical instrument. The data have to be stored and retrieved in a
way that makes spatial sense. When I think about the way we assess this memory
clinically, the tests are generally crude and two-dimensional. Real live
visuospatial memory is much different. As an example I can visualize being in
an English class at the northwest corner of my high school. I know the exit I
would have to take and the stairwell to get out of the building. I know the
directions to get to my locker and I can run through all of this traveling in
my head. At some level that is a declarative memory function because I know the
information I want, I can retrieve it, and I can do an operation on it and in this
case imagine myself traveling according to that information. Why my brain
elects to access visuospatial information while I am dreaming is not clear and
I don’t think anyone knows the answer to that question.
Early information from lesion studies of parietal cortex
was involved in visuospatial memories especially the right parietal cortex. That was largely because patients with
lesions in this area could not complete the basic construction tasks on bedside
cognitive screening. Parietal cortex is important in the subjective experience
of memory. For example patient with bilateral
parietal cortex lesions have similar recall as controls but they have less
confidence in their recall (2). One of
the most well-known studies of visuospatial recall and brain structural changes
was a study of London cab drivers. They
are required to acquire a significant amount of visuospatial information about
London geography and be tested on it. In the initial study (3) the authors showed that
acquiring this information was associated with greater cross sectional area of
the right posterior hippocampus on MRI scan.
They suggested: “These data are in accordance with the idea that the
posterior hippocampus stores a spatial representation of the environment and
can expand regionally to accommodate elaboration of this representation in
people with a high dependence on navigational skills.” In a recent study of typical (non-expert) taxi
drivers navigational skills did not seem to correlate with right posterior hippocampal
volume (5). One of the explanations that
the authors had for this issue is that the posterior hippocampus is only one
part of the structures necessary for the navigational map.
Since this thread of research on the hippocampus, the area
of spatial learning has become a lot more complex. As an example, there are now three different
types of spatial learning (6). Landmark
knowledge is specific for objects encountered in the environment. Route knowledge involves
environmentally dependent decisions that have to occur at specific times to
move through the environment. Survey
knowledge is a detailed representation of the relationship between all
objects in the environment. In reference 6, the authors look at testing all of
these types of spatial learning in a large group of people who are navigating a
nuclear facility and looking at the impact that maps of varying complexity have
on their learning that environment.
As a psychiatrist, one of my concerns is what the neural substrate
of spatial learning is for both my own thought experiments but also the patients I see with known brain lesions or who have clear deficits in spatial
navigation. One of the most useful
reviews I could find (7) looked at a summary of the available data and proposed
a navigational network that is really a rough sketch. In a fairly complex paper the authors suggest
that the occipital place area (OPA), retrosplenial complex (RSA) and
parahippocampal place area (PPA) may function for landmark recognition in the
context of a larger network involving the hippocampus, parahippocampus,
retrosplenial cortex, and entorhinal cortex.
From a clinical standpoint many of these structures are compromised in
dementias like Alzheimer Disease with severe associated problems in spatial
maps and orientation.
The story of spatial mapping and memory is an incomplete
one at this time, but the details are building. At some point in the near
future we will have a better idea of what the brain substrate is for this
memory and the various kinds of real or imagined navigation. How it is activated
and placed in dreams will probably take additional research effort. And at some point, I really would like to see a practical approach to estimating the information content and flow necessary to accomplish these tasks.
The most important aspect of spatial representations may be the emergent properties of the system. Consciousness scientist Christof Koch (11) has stated that there are an "uncountable" number of spatial relationships associated with any human experience. Integrated into those spatial relationships are also sensory and affective dimensions. That results in a powerful system for recall of real physical systems or building new ones in your dreams.
George Dawson, MD, DFAPA
References:
1: Rosen ML, Stern
CE, Devaney KJ, Somers DC. Cortical and Subcortical Contributions to Long-Term
Memory-Guided Visuospatial Attention. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y. : 1991).
2018 Aug;28(8):2935-2947. DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhx172.
2: Simons JS, Peers
PV, Mazuz YS, Berryhill ME, Olson IR. Dissociation between memory accuracy and
memory confidence following bilateral parietal lesions. Cereb Cortex.
2010;20(2):479-485. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhp116
3: Maguire EA, Gadian DG, Johnsrude IS, et al.
Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2000;97(8):4398-4403. doi:10.1073/pnas.070039597
4: Maguire EA,
Woollett K, Spiers HJ. London taxi drivers and bus drivers: a structural MRI
and neuropsychological analysis. Hippocampus.
2006;16(12):1091-1101. doi:10.1002/hipo.20233
5: Weisberg SM,
Newcombe NS, Chatterjee A. Everyday taxi drivers: Do better navigators have
larger hippocampi?. Cortex. 2019;115:280-293. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2018.12.024
10: Eichenbaum H. The role of the hippocampus in navigation is memory. J Neurophysiol. 2017;117(4):1785-1796. doi:10.1152/jn.00005.2017
11: Christof Koch. The Feeling of Life Itself. MIT Press. Cambridge, MA, 2019. p. 7-8.
Friday, July 17, 2020
Toward A Better Sedative Hypnotic Medication
I made the mistake of suggesting that we need a better version of quetiapine for insomnia on Twitter. That triggered the usual comments that basically suggested that I did not know anything about quetiapine or what I was doing in general. This post is for cooler heads. Let me start out by saying quetiapine came out early in my career. The early concern was ocular side effects and of course cardiac conduction problems (prolonged QTc). Since that time the ocular side effects are not a primary concern (both retinal hyperpigmentation and cataracts were a concern with phenothiazines). Since I was in acute care psychiatry, the main concern was that this medication did not work fast enough for acute mania or psychosis. There was a time lag of days to weeks relative to more potent atypical antipsychotics (AAP). And not too long after they were released the metabolic problems were noted including dyslipidemia, insulin resistance and overt diabetes mellitus, metabolic syndrome, and weight gain. Since most Americans are overweight and have a moderate risk of diabetes mellitus Type 2 in middle age – this is the primary reason to avoid using most medications in this class. Theoretical considerations suggest that the newer AAPs – aripiprazole, lurasidone, and brexpiprazole have less risk, but I have certainly seen people who gained substantial amounts of weight on these agents. I have also seen several people develop diabetes without gaining a pound. That risk is clearly there and it is significant.
With the exception of the above orexin receptor antagonists, amitriptyline, and trimipramine, I use all of the compounds in the above table for the treatment of insomnia. The most commonly used medication is trazodone. It is generally well tolerated and effective. Like all medications it is not 100% effective and most common reasons people request changes are excessive morning sedation, daytime drowsiness, excessive dreaming, and nasal congestion. The remaining sedating antidepressants all depend heavily on anti-histaminergic effects enter probably as well tolerated but have unique side effects that need to be monitored. The only antidepressant that has an FDA indication for sleep is doxepin and that is in a proprietary dosage form of 3 and 6 mg. According to the Ki values, doxepin is the highest affinity for H1 receptors. Insurance company constraints usually result in people getting low-dose generic doxepin rather than the FDA approved proprietary version Silenor. Silenor comes in 3 mg and 6 mg tabs and the smallest generic dose of doxepin that I can typically prescribe is 10 mg.
In the clinical population I see practically everyone has access to melatonin. It appears to be much less effective than trazodone but has the appeal that it is a “natural” supplement. That also means it is not monitored by the FDA in terms of bioequivalence. The dose of melatonin that most people take is many times the amount that is produce physiologically. I was told by one of the top sleep experts in the country several years ago that the appropriate way to take it is taking 1 or 2 mg at five or 6 PM to mimic the physiological release of melatonin. I often make that suggestion people who tell me they feel like they are taking too much with a 5 to 10 mg dose at bedtime. Ramelteon is available as melatonin receptor agonist and seems to be effective for sleep onset in about 30% of the people I see.
In the case of the second response, I get very interesting histories of severe treatment refractory insomnia associated with substance use. Most people take 25-150 mg of quetiapine for sleep but some take doses that are typically used for the stabilization of bipolar disorder (300-800 mg at night). And they have been taking it for years. My basic job is to make sure they do not have tardive dyskinesia, diabetes mellitus, or cardiac conduction problems but things can get even more complicated than that. For example, what about the person who already has diabetes mellitus Type II, hypertension, dyslipidemia, heart disease and a severe alcohol use problem? What if they tell you: “Look doc – if I can’t use quetiapine for sleep my go to is alcohol and I know that alcohol is killing me. You have to let me use that quetiapine or I will end up drinking myself to death”.
Supplementary:
Table added to clarify the muscarinic acetylcholine receptor (mAChR) functions and the subtype most implicated in delirium. (click to enlarge).
References:
9. Hshieh TT, Fong TG, Marcantonio ER, Inouye SK. Cholinergic deficiency hypothesis in delirium: a synthesis of current evidence. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2008;63(7):764-772. doi:10.1093/gerona/63.7.764
Monday, July 13, 2020
Airborne Transmission Denial Dies Hard ........
I started this post as I left a staff meeting today on containing the coronoavirus. We had a similar meeting 2 months ago and at that point I added that there was airborne transmission of the virus. The only comment I got was a condescending remark about how we don't know much about airborne transmission and we need to wait and see and blah, bah, blah. It was "as if" I did not know what I was talking about or any of the surrounding controversy. To my surprise the same people today were sold on airborne transmission. They were even interested in HVAC issues and negative pressure rooms – all of the stuff I have been studying for 20 years. Nobody mentioned UVC or air filtration. I decided just to keep my mouth shut. Just like I usually do when politics seems to be the priority rather than science. But the good news was undeniable. Airborne transmission has much greater acceptance than it did prior to the current pandemic and there are clear reasons for it.
Previous Airborne Transmission Posts from this Blog:
SARS-CoV-2 Is An Airborne Virus?
Viruses Are In The Air - Protection From Airborne Viruses
Hand Washing
New Twist On An Old Method To Kill The Flu Virus
Is It Time To Quarantine Air Travelers?
Supplementary:
This statement from a recent Nature article:
"But this conclusion is not popular with some experts because it goes against decades of thinking about respiratory infections. Since the 1930s, public-health researchers and officials have generally discounted the importance of aerosols — droplets less than 5 micrometres in diameter — in respiratory diseases such as influenza."
from: Dyani Lewis. Mounting evidence suggests coronavirus is airborne — but health advice has not caught up. Link.
Graphics Credit:
Monday, June 29, 2020
Should You Trust Your Physician?
As far as I can tell there are two basic considerations in whether or not you trust your physician. The first is combination of personality and cultural factors and the resulting expectations you have when you see a physician. That may have something to do with your actual experience, but also may have more to do with observations from the care of other people. As an example, you saw your father was cared for and thought it was excellent care and expect the same care for yourself. That can also backfire in the case where you believe the observed care was substandard and led you to be more skeptical of medical care administered by physicians. From a cultural standpoint, you may be from a culture that does not trust authority figures or even physicians. These are all very complicated issues, that I will illustrate with personal examples of treatment I have received over the years.
George Dawson, MD, DFAPA