Showing posts with label reviving behavior. Show all posts
Showing posts with label reviving behavior. Show all posts

Monday, March 10, 2025

Mouse-to-Mouse Resuscitation

 



A couple of papers came out in Science 2 weeks ago on the behavior of mice toward their unconscious or dead peers.  They have resuscitation-like behavior that consists of biting the face and tongue, clearing the airway, elevating the tongue of the unconscious mouse to revive them. Mice who were the recipients of this behavior recovered sooner than mice who did not receive these efforts.  The authors went on to see if they could localize the behavior in the brain and using modern neuroscience techniques, they able to show that they had localized the behavior to a brain substrate.  These papers have implications for psychiatry both at a theoretical and practical level.

In this experiment researchers used genetically identical mice (cross bred for 20 generations).  They followed their reactions closely across time epochs as they encountered a familiar partner anesthetized and unresponsive or euthanized (dead) mice. When compared with the active partner, mice spend 47.4% of the time interacting with the unresponsive partner compared to 5.8% of the time with the active partner.  They also displayed a specific pattern of behaviors directed facial area, trunk, limbs and head of the unresponsive partner. 31.8% of the time was directed at the orofacial area.  The overall duration of contact was extended with the unresponsive partner and it was more focused on the orofacial area.

To confirm the sequence of events, short acting (isoflurane) anesthesia was used to observe the partner response as the anesthetized mouse became less and then more responsive.  Time spent interacting with the anesthetized partner and the topology of interactions (more targeted orofacial activity correlated with the time anesthetized.  Deceased cagemates caused the same pattern of interaction but not sleeping cagemates because during sleep the directed behaviors led to movements of the sleeping mouse. This suggests that mice can differentiate responsive and unresponsive states in their partners.

The orofacial focused behaviors were studied under a high-speed camera.  It was discovered that in nonresponsive mice the tongue was bitten and pulled out of the mouth by casemates.  A similar tongue protrusion did not occur in unresponsive mice.  Pulling the tongue out resulted in a larger airway and the removal of foreign objects in the mouth. Placing the foreign object in a non-oral orifice did not result in removal. Stimulating the oral mucosa was also a strong stimulus for the righting response and arousal. 

Familiarity was a stronger stimulus for eliciting the grooming and resuscitation like behaviors than sex differences or similarities.  When given a choice between familiar partners – one anesthetized and one not – there was a preference for the anesthetized partner.  That did not persist when both target mice were unfamiliar.  The authors also demonstrated that the time spent with the unresponsive mouse did not decrease over a period of days – it did not show habituation.

After characterizing the unique behavioral aspects of the resuscitation like behavior the authors looked at the neural substrate. One hour after exposure to the unresponsive mice the cagemates were given 4-hydroxytamoxifen to label activated neurons.  Following 2 weeks of exposure several brain structures previously implicated in the observed behaviors including the medial amygdalar nucleus (MEA), paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus (PVH), basolateral amygdalar nucleus (BLA), hippocampus, and ventromedial hypothalamic nucleus (VMH) were examined. The number of tdTomato+ cells were noted in mice that had responded to unresponsive mice compared with controls (active partners).  This marker is a sign of the transcription factor c-fos and that neurons were recently active. Using a second set of probes roughly twice as many PVH oxytocin neurons were c-fos + and oxytocin + in the mice exposed to an unresponsive partner. 

An additional optogenetics experiment was done to look at the oxytocin PVH neurons.  Optogenetically silenced PVH neuron had the expected effect of reducing interaction time with the unresponsive partner. Optogenetically activating PVH neurons had the effect of increasing interaction time with an unresponsive stranger mouse.  An oxytocin receptor antagonist has the expected effect of reducing interaction time with the unresponsive mouse.

The authors conclude that they have demonstrated the necessity of both an oxytocin neuronal substrate and oxytocin signaling for very specific interactions and behaviors toward unresponsive mice.  Further, encountering a dead or anesthetized partner is required to elicit this response but it does not occur with a sleeping partner. They point out that rapid responses to an unresponsive partner can reduce the time it takes for recovery and decreases the risk of predation.  They suggest further work needs to be done on defining the neural network and sensory cues necessary for these behaviors.

As I read this paper I had a few thoughts:

1:  The hypothalamus is underemphasized in psychiatry.  We spent a couple of decades studying neuroendocrinology that was primarily focused on the pituitary gland. In clinical work, this is also an important system to be able to assess. But in much of the work about the theoretical basis of behavior – not much is said about the hypothalamus.  

2:  Social behavior versus the neurobiological substrate – psychosocial reductionists frequently treat the brain like a tableau rasa - not much is there until some kind of social learning or adverse event occurs.  In this case, adaptive complicated behavior is observed without any training.  The experiment illustrates both an anatomical and neurochemical basis for the behavior. Why would we expect anything to be different in humans?

3:  The measurements of activity in both the resuscitation-like and non-resuscitation behaviors were interesting.  Even though the overt behaviors were easily observed as occurring or not occurring – the neurons involved always had some level of activity. In other words – no target behavior did not equate to no neuronal activity.  It is not simply a case of networks being on or off. This has implications for how we attempt to correlate networks with behavior and the meaning of networks having a certain level of baseline activity.

All things considered; I thought this was a great paper. It reminded me of my biochemistry and pharmacology seminars in medical school where we would have spent a lot more time on the experimental methodology in this paper.  I did check my latest copy of The Molecular Biology of the Cell (7th edition) and found that the discussion of optogenetics relative to this paper was very brief, but I suppose that makes sense.  From the standpoint of animal behavior this also recalls so-called altruistic behavior of some animals.  I have a file of that behavior observed in Humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) interfering with killer whales (Orcinus orca) and hope to post something about that as well.

 

George Dawson, MD, DFAPA

 

References:

1:  Sun W, Zhang GW, Huang JJ, Tao C, Seo MB, Tao HW, Zhang LI. Reviving-like prosocial behavior in response to unconscious or dead conspecifics in rodents. Science. 2025 Feb 21;387(6736):eadq2677. doi: 10.1126/science.adq2677. Epub 2025 Feb 21. PMID: 39977514.

2:  DeNardo L, Luo L. Genetic strategies to access activated neurons. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2017 Aug;45:121-129. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2017.05.014. Epub 2017 May 31. PMID: 28577429; PMCID: PMC5810937.


Image Credit:

Generated by ChatGPT based on my parameters.  The original paper has a great image of the specific behaviors mentioned in this post.