A brief news report in Science this week by Kelly Servick provides a good discussion of the adherence issue from a number of perspectives. The central graphic is from a paper by Blashke, et al (2) showing summary data of electronic monitoring of medication adherence from 95 clinical trials that shows decreased adherence rates in terms of taking the medication and taking the medication as prescribed. Both fall off significantly over time. By 100 days 20% of subjects have stopped taking the medication and about 30% are no longer taking it as prescribed. Those are substantial numbers especially if the active drug can be identified by a specific effect or side effect and discontinued on that basis. In a field where there is a significant placebo response among subjects with mild to moderate illness non-adherence can lead to significant problems in the final outcome and overall worth of the study.
According to Servick the typical approach used in the past has been to recruit enough subjects to counter the low adherence rates. This is problematic for a number of reasons. Subjects these days are often from nonclinical samples. On college campuses this can be a problem with some subjects volunteering for multiple studies. With psychiatric drug trials, the recruitment criteria are subjective, obvious, and selection is often coordinated by non-physician research coordinators whose job it is to get the required number of volunteers in a specified period of time. In the drug trials that I am personally aware of only Alzheimer's disease trials asked for corroboration from sources other than the patients on how they were able to function on a daily basis. It would be very interesting to obtain that kind of data on subjects recruited from University campuses who were still attending classes especially if some incentive was involved. In a related matter, one of the investigators in this area created a database to identify potential subjects who came in for screening at various sites where he was an investigator. Up to 7.78% of the subjects across 9 sites were identified as duplicates (3). Because of the potential negative effect of duplicate subjects the authors suggest that a nation wide database of subject should be considered.
The article looks at a number of measures to determine the level of adherence in a study. The first take home message is that pill counts are relatively meaningless. I have certainly talked with research subjects who told me that their blister packs were empty because they just threw the pills away. In a study that compared pills counts with blood levels of the drug the sample sample had an adherence rate of 92% by pill count but only 70% by blood levels. The author cites medication side effects as a reason for non-adherence, but in a complex sample of patients with varying levels of motivation and insight the reasons can be very complex. Several electronic approaches to adherence have been devised that vary from a chip in the pill bottle cap that records when the bottle is opened and closed (MEMS system) to a chip in a pill that records when it is ingested.
Adherence measures are another dimension to look for when reading the results of clinical trials. I don't recall seeing any commentary on this important issue in Cochrane reviews and probably with good reason. Non-adherence rates this high are probably at least as important as what Cochrane typically discusses as technical problems like small sample size and measurement problems. Blaschke is quoted in the Servick article that many of the researchers in this area feel that the problem is bigger than one that can be detected by surveillance and databases. To me this comes down to the limitations of clinical trials and a problem that cannot be potentially solved. Certainly the days of research units where subjects could be supervised in inpatient settings for months is gone. In most cases, persons with severe psychiatric disorders can only get that kind of treatment if they can personally pay for it or the state they live in has a state psychiatric facility. Even then they often have to undergo civil commitment. A practical solution would be to eliminate the obviously non-adherent subjects and not include them in any intent-to-treat analysis and use a standard adherence measure such as blood levels where appropriate. Ambivalence about taking a drug in a research protocol is not the same thing as stopping an FDA approved drug in a clinical setting, but that conscious state has not been adequately studied.
George Dawson, MD, DFAPA
Supplementary 1: In researching this article I was very pleased to find the full text of Blaschke, et al online but also a reference to the National Academy of Sciences Committee on National Statistics. That site contains a report The Prevention and Treatment of Missing Data in Clinical Trials that was references in the original Science article. The full article can be obtained at no cost form that site with registration.
Supplementary 1: In researching this article I was very pleased to find the full text of Blaschke, et al online but also a reference to the National Academy of Sciences Committee on National Statistics. That site contains a report The Prevention and Treatment of Missing Data in Clinical Trials that was references in the original Science article. The full article can be obtained at no cost form that site with registration.
4: Czobor P, Skolnick P. The secrets of a successful clinical trial: compliance, compliance, and compliance. Mol Interv. 2011 Apr;11(2):107-10. doi: 10.1124/mi.11.2.8. PubMed PMID: 21540470; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3109858.
5: General search on adherence related articles