Friday, February 15, 2019
Medical Care Of The Seriously Mentally Ill - The Way It Should Be Provided
I was impressed with the week's Case Records of the Massachusetts General Hospital in the New England Journal of Medicine for a couple of reasons. First, it seems to be a continuation of a renewed effort to cover psychiatry and the medical interface. This has been done before. I used to track how many references to psychiatry or psychiatric symptoms there were in this feature. Over about 20 years - I think I came up with 5-10 references per year. The discussants were generally not psychiatrists because the symptoms originated from neurological or medical disorders. This case is a patient with obvious psychiatric illness occurring in the common context of an inadequate history and no collateral information. Secondly. it discusses how care should be provided to these patients and in today's reality of severely rationed care, that is extremely important. My goal with this post is to point out some of the lessons that I think are important from this case, based on 22 years of experience providing the same level of care that the authors do here. The case itself is presented in detail and I will not repeat those details here.
The patient in this case is a 48-year-old woman who presents to the emergency department with "prickling and tingling" in the palm of her right hand. She presented about 5 hours after she had previously been discharged for evaluation of the same problem. She had been discharged with a list of shelters and hotels but came back saying that she was concerned that the hotel staff would "gas" her or perform "unwanted sleep studies" on her. The main case discussants were psychiatrists who presented all of the review of systems, mental status exam findings, physical findings, and laboratory findings. She had a normal physical exam and the mental status exam was remarkable for mild cognitive impairment on a standard cognitive screen, psychosis in the form of loosely organized paranoid delusions, grandiosity, pressured speech, and labile affect. The patient also had impaired judgment and because of the concern about her ability to care for herself-she was admitted on an involuntary basis.
The differential diagnosis of the psychiatric disorder from the perspective of a primary or secondary psychosis is discussed in detail. Nine major categories of disease are covered as well as specific possibilities in each category. The discussions include commentary on why a particular diagnosis is likely or not. Following that discussion the psychiatrist presents the most likely psychiatric and physical diagnosis to account for the presentation. Reading through this exercise, I thought that the discussion was outstanding and potentially much more enlightening that a vague abbreviated continuing education exercise. I recommend that residents read through this exercise and think carefully about the decision points presented by the psychiatrist-discussant. I will add that my inpatient colleagues and I have made the diagnoses given in this proposed two diagnosis list many times. We also began screening for the medical problem in the exact same way suggested in this article starting about 20 years ago. Anyone interested in this process should read this article in detail. I consider it to be a great example of the medical thinking and skills required to be a psychiatrist.
A hematologist also discussed the findings including explaining nuances of the laboratory results and confirmed the diagnosis.
An added bonus in this case was a discussion of the difficulties in making medical diagnoses in psychiatric patients with severe mental illnesses by Dr. Freudenreich. He identified the difficulty in the interview situation as a lack of reliable information and no collateral information generally available in emergency situations. He used a term "diagnostic overshadowing" or attributing physical symptoms to a psychiatric disease (the old "it's all in your head"). He suggests a three question approach to avoid that kind of problem by taking into account the psychiatric diagnosis, whether a reasonable evaluation of common problems has been done, and whether or not the psychiatric illness contributes to the medical problem. In my experience, I have seen flat out denial that a problem exists - ranging from diabetes mellitus to lung cancer to be a major obstacle to both acute and ongoing care.
Another aspect of appropriate care of populations with severe psychiatric disorders is the ability to establish dialogue with that person. Psychiatrists are used to talking with people who are delusional, hallucinating, and who have formal thought disorders. A lot of physicians do not have that experience or interest and that results in truncated interviews and not much exchange of information. Another aspect of the psychiatric interview is that the psychiatrist is always making determinations about whether what is being said by the patient is really accurate or whether it is distorted or symbolic communication. That process helps to determine what the physical concerns are and to what extent they need to be evaluated.
Providing care based on the inability to care for oneself rather than dangerousness is a key part of this case. At one point the authors point out that the patient was admitted on an involuntary basis. Subsequent to that admission she was diagnosed and definitively treated for a condition that is at the minimum severely disabling and in the worst case fatal. She had been released from a previous emergency department. I can say without a doubt that she would have been released from any emergency department in the state where I currently work. Minnesota has been living the myth that there is a "shortage" of psychiatric beds for about 30 years now without acknowledging that the state government is directly responsible for the shortage. One of the commonest rationales for discharging people with severe mental illness and vague medical complaints to the street is that they are not "dangerous". Like every other state there is a gravely disabled standard for emergency holds that allows for the involuntary admission based on disability due to mental illness. Like every other state - Minnesota ignores it.
In summary this case report is much more than an exercise in the differential diagnosis of psychiatric and medical disorders. This case report represents a standard of care for persons with severe mental illness. Any reader of this post who is familiar with the lack of standards of care for this population in the US healthcare system doesn't have to imagine that this person could have been repeatedly discharged from numerous emergency departments, sent to jail for trespassing, or given a bus ticket to the next town. Imagination is not required because we have all seen it happen. There are numerous rationalizations, but saving money by not caring these folks is the overriding factor. Health care systems, state governments, and county governments have all turned their backs on them. In the midwest, the only time the homeless get a break is when the temperature drops to 20 below and decisions are made to free up more resources so they don't die on the streets. This case illustrates that a warmer environment is no less hostile if you can't get the medical care that you need.
Kudos to the psychiatrists and physicians providing this level of care wherever they can and the NEJM for raising awareness on this issue. The larger societal issue is why this level of care is not available everywhere and why persons with mental illness cannot expect the same level of care as medical or surgical patients.
George Dawson, MD, DFAPA
Reference:
Hogan C, Little BP, Carlson JCT, Freudenreich O, Ivkovic A, Baron JM. Case Records of the Massachusetts General Hospital: A Woman With Delusional Thinking and Paresthesia of the Right Hand. N Engl J Med 2019; 380: 665-674. Link
Other Case Records of the Massachusetts General Hospital:
Shtasel DL, Freudenreich O, Baggett TP. Case Records of the Massachusetts General Hospital: Case 40-2015. A 40-Year-Old Homeless Woman with Headache, Hypertension, and Psychosis. N Engl J Med. 2015 Dec 24;373(26):2563-70. doi: 10.1056/NEJMcpc1405204. PubMed PMID: 26699172.
Irwin KE, Freudenreich O, Peppercorn J, Taghian AG, Freer PE, Gudewicz TM. Case Records of the Massachusetts General Hospital: Case 30-2016. A 63-Year-Old Woman with Bipolar Disorder, Cancer, and Worsening Depression. N Engl J Med. 2016 Sep 29;375(13):1270-81. doi: 10.1056/NEJMcpc1609309. PubMed PMID: 27682037.
Graphics Credit:
Graphic of the organometallic compound that is the key to this case was downloaded from PubChem.
Monday, February 11, 2019
Stochastic Processes In Human Biology
I was reading an important research article recently and encountered a term that I had not see in a while. That word was stochastic. Physical science and engineering undergraduates probably encounter these words with different frequencies - most often as the expression stochastic process. As a chemistry and biology major, the main contact I had with the term was in what is typically considered the most mathematical of undergraduate chemistry courses - Physical Chemistry (PChem). In the days I was an undergraduate we used a text by Moore. In striving to stay in touch with the field I got a copy of a more current PChem text by Atkins several years ago. The main reference to stochastic processes in both is the random walk problem. In Moore it is in the problem set for the chapter on Kinetic Theory and and in Atkins it is in a separate supplement at the end. That supplement is entitled Random Walk, illustrates how a random walk in one dimension can be used to derive an equation that estimates the probability of a particle being at a distance from the origin in a specified time during diffusion. I won't include the equation here since there are a number of books and online sites where the derivation is available.
The authors of the research article were using it to describe a component of neuronal activity in the reward center of rats. In their discussion of the term, they point out that brain complexity precludes the prediction of final outcomes from initial states. In the paper the authors were faced with explaining why a specific population of mice continued to self-stimulate dopaminergic neurons in the ventral tegmental area (VTA) via a optogenetic dopamine- neuron self-stimulation (oDASS) via an optic fiber despite punishment while another group of mice did not. The details are included in the graphic at the top of this post. To quote the authors on this phenomenon:
"Why only a fraction of mice lose control remains to be determined; the emergence of the two groups is even more surprising given the high degree of genetic homogeneity of the mouse line used here (our Datcre (also known as Slc6a3cre) mice were backcrossed for more than ten generations into the C57BL/6J mouse line), and may reflect a case of stochastic individuality" (p. 320)
One of the key elements of this type of experimentation is minimizing variance form genetic and environmental factors. The mice used in this experiment are inbred for several generations and in this case had a homozygous knock-in variation for dopamine transporter (DAT). Colonies are developed to raise heterozygous mice with a greater expression of DAT for experimentation on these systems. The idea behind backcrossing the mice for more than ten generations reduces the phenotypic variation but as noted in reference 2, there have been 30 years of experimentation of inbreeding mice and that reduces the phenotypic variance by 20-30%. Although the C57BL/6J mouse line has its DNA characterized - I could not find any numbers for shared DNA on an individual to individual mouse basis. As an example, in humans the following percentages of shared DNA would be expected identical twins (100%), parent-child (50%), grandparent-grandchild (25%), and first cousins (7.3-13.8%).
The experiment looked at these mice bred for a low degree of phenotypic variation who were all rained in similar environments. All of the mice were trained for oDASS and then subjected to foot shock as a punishment. As noted in the graphic, 60% persevered despite the punishment and 40% renounced or had a marked decrease in oDASS after punishment. Both of these responses were considered distinct behavioral phenotypes. The underlying molecular mechanisms were also studied.
The paper by Honegger and de Bivort (2) takes a more detailed look at examples of stochasticity. From a behavioral standpoint, they define stochastic individuality as non-heritable inter-organism behavioral variation. The lack of concordance between identical twins is cited as a prime example. Beyond genomics they argue that stochastic individuality implies that if we know all about the basis of a trait at the -omics level and all of the relevant environmental factors – the behaviors will remain "beyond reliable prediction". They review some examples from the animal kingdom. Marbled crayfish and pea aphids reproduce by apomictic thelytoky - that is all of the individuals are female and reproduction is clonal - therefore all individuals have the same genome. Despite this individual display significant variation in locomotion and social behavior. They consider nutritional variances, self reinforcing circuits involving nutrition and behavior, and variation in biophysical developmental processes as possible mechanisms. In the case of mice, highly inbred mice raised in the same environment vary significantly in their exploration of the environment. Individual mice that actively explore the environment have more robust hippocampal neurogenesis.
The authors detail and number of possible mechanisms underlying stochastic individuality including:
1. A small number of effects at the molecular level promotes stochasticity. Random fluctuations in RNA polymerase binding to DNA over time leads to fluctuating mRNA in the cell over time. There are also a host of possible mechanisms operating on small gene sets that lead to large combinations of possible outcomes. An example given is T-cell biology in humans. T-cells are essential for normal immunity in humans and the system can use a number of genetic mechanisms to generate a a very large number of T-cell receptor types (1015 to 1020 ) from a small number of genes (4). The authors of this article discuss the fact that T-cell receptor diversity cannot be estimated by a priori assumptions suggesting this is a stochastic rather than deterministic process.
2. Positive feedback in gene networks amplify fluctuations and can cause jumping between discrete states or bistability. An example from the literature is cell quiescence, and there is thought to be a stochastic process involving a specific protein (5,6) that leads to the transition from active proliferation ( a number of states) to quiescence and back again.
3. Biological processes are systems that are non-linear, multidimensional, and have significant feedback and therefore are often chaotic. Initial small differences will be amplified. There are several disorders that occur with different concordance in identical twins (seizure disorders, cardiac arrhythmias) that could occur as the consequence of this process. Biological systems that move large amounts of information across these systems are most susceptible.
4. Somatic mosaicism – as cellular differentiation and proliferation occur genomic alteration can occur in new cells or their progeny. Transposons are active mobile DNA elements that can excise and re-insert into new positions in the genome leading to different cell genetics and tissues from the same progenitor cells. In my current neurobiology lecture I discuss various level of brain complexity and give examples of 90 subtypes of potassium channels and significant numbers of vesicle trafficking proteins (7) in neurons. Wilhelm, et al (7) reconstruct a 3D model of a synapse that contains 60 different in varying copy numbers. In the supplementary data they investigate a total of 100 different synaptic proteins and show that the copy numbers of each protein vary from 10 to 22,000 (Fig. S5).
The complexity of the synaptic proteins and their number may seem impressive and have the complexity to produce an apparent stochastic result. Looking at what might happen if these proteins are all modifiable by one of the mechanisms in the Honegger and de Bivort paper is more impressive. The authors discuss alternative gene splice forms using a Drosophila example. Proteins are typically formed in eukaryotic cells when the exon portions of the gene are cut and spliced from the intron or noncoding segment of a DNA or corresponding RNA transcript. Alternative splicing involves splicing that occurs in a way that some segments are altered or skipped. The result is multiple proteins being encoded by the same gene. A few examples are illustrated in the following graphic.
The nervous system implications of alternative splicing include recognizing that this occurs at every level of neuronal organization (8) and it has already been implicated in neurological diseases like amyotropic lateral sclerosis (ALS). To get a quick look at what research may have already been done in this area I surveyed Medline for references to "alternative splicing" and "protein isoforms" for all of the neuronal proteins listed in the table of neuronal proteins listed in table S2 from reference 7. As noted in the table there are probably over a thousand references unevenly distributed across these proteins indicating a significant amount of research on both the mechanism and classification of neuronal protein isoforms - many of which impact neuronal function and may be responsible for stochastic outcomes.
Research On Neuronal
Protein Alternative Splicing and Isoforms
|
||
Protein
|
Alternative Splicing
references?
|
"Protein
Isoforms"[MeSH Terms] + Protein references?
|
SNAP 25
|
+
|
+
|
VAMP 2
|
+
|
+
|
α-SNAP
|
+
|
+
|
α/β-Synuclein
|
+
|
+
|
AP 180
|
+
|
+
|
AP 2 (mu2)
|
+
|
+
|
CALM
|
+
|
+
|
Calmodulin
|
+
|
+
|
Clathrin heavy chain
|
+
|
+
|
Clathrin light chain
|
+
|
+
|
Complexin 1,2
|
-
|
+
|
CSP
|
-
|
+
|
Doc2A/B
|
-
|
-
|
Dynamin 1,2,3
|
-
|
+
|
Endophilin I,II,III
|
-
|
+
|
Epsin 1
|
-
|
+
|
Hsc70
|
-
|
+
|
Intersectin 1
|
+
|
+
|
Munc13a
|
-
|
-
|
Munc18a
|
-
|
+
|
NSF
|
+
|
+
|
PIPKIγ
|
-
|
+
|
Rab3
|
+
|
+
|
Rab5a
|
-
|
+
|
Rab7a
|
-
|
+
|
Septin5
|
+
|
+
|
SGIP1
|
-
|
-
|
Synapsin I.II
|
+
|
+
|
Syndapin 1
|
+
|
+
|
At this point I am wrapping up this post with a list of stochastic mechanisms to follow in subsequent posts. This is an important concept for psychiatrists interested in individual differences in behavior and well as unique conscious states to know about. It has the potential for greatly increasing the explanatory power of why there is so much heterogeneity in presentations of illness and outcomes. It has the potential for providing a clearer understanding of the neurobiological substrates of behavior. The distinct behavioral phenotypes in the experiment noted in the beginning of this post is a clear case in point.
George Dawson, MD, DFAPA
References:
1: Pascoli V, Hiver A, Van Zessen R, Loureiro M, Achargui R, Harada M, Flakowski J, Lüscher C. Stochastic synaptic plasticity underlying compulsion in a model of addiction. Nature. 2018 Dec;564(7736):366-371. doi: 10.1038/s41586-018-0789-4. Epub 2018 Dec 19. PubMed PMID: 30568192.
3: Ponomarenko EA, Poverennaya EV, Ilgisonis EV, Pyatnitskiy MA, Kopylov AT, Zgoda VG, Lisitsa AV, Archakov AI. The Size of the Human Proteome: The Width and Depth. Int J Anal Chem. 2016;2016:7436849. doi: 10.1155/2016/7436849. Epub 2016 May 19. Review. PubMed PMID: 27298622.
4: Laydon DJ, Bangham CR, Asquith B. Estimating T-cell repertoire diversity:limitations of classical estimators and a new approach. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2015 Aug 19;370(1675). pii: 20140291. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2014.0291. Review. PubMed PMID: 26150657.
7: Wilhelm BG, Mandad S, Truckenbrodt S, Kröhnert K, Schäfer C, Rammner B, KooSJ, Claßen GA, Krauss M, Haucke V, Urlaub H, Rizzoli SO. Composition of isolated synaptic boutons reveals the amounts of vesicle trafficking proteins. Science. 2014 May 30;344(6187):1023-8. doi: 10.1126/science.1252884. PubMed PMID:24876496.
8: Vuong CK, Black DL, Zheng S. The neurogenetics of alternative splicing. NatRev Neurosci. 2016 May;17(5):265-81. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.27. Review. PubMedPMID: 27094079.
Supplementary:
The Kyoto Encyclopedia of Genes and Genomes (KEGG) currently lists 40 alternative splicing factors. Link
Graphics Credit:
1: Top graphic -> me.
2: Alternate splicing graphic from VisiScience per their purchasing agreement.
Click on either graphic to enlarge.
1: Top graphic -> me.
2: Alternate splicing graphic from VisiScience per their purchasing agreement.
Click on either graphic to enlarge.
Tuesday, January 29, 2019
The Laparoscopic Cholecystectomy
I had a laparoscopic cholecystectomy done on January 28. The indication was possible biliary colic and atypical symptoms of gallstone disease and a gallstone and polyp in the gallbladder noted on abdominal ultrasound. That scan was done to determine a possible cause of 2 years of bloating and tachycardia. There was no right upper quadrant pain or colic following fatty foods. The confounder there is that I hardly eat any fatty foods. All of my dairy products are fat free. I have not eaten beef in over 30 years and the only time I eat bacon is as a condiment. I know the cultural swing in medicine is that fat and cholesterol are now supposed to be "good" for you but I don't buy that and the proof is my fasting lipid profile. I also recently learned that two of my cousins had cholecystectomies at a young age for gallbladder disease so there may be a genetic factor. The diagnosis did take me by surprise and might illustrate the value of retrospective analysis.
When I presented to my internist with symptoms of abdominal bloating and postprandial tachycardia in the absence of any blood test abnormalities it suggested to him that it was more of a dumping syndrome except there were not associated symptoms or diseases. He set up the ultrasound and called me with the results. Surgical referral was next. The surgeon suggested that while I did not have "classic" symptoms of gallbladder disease that he had seen varied presentations over the years including the symptoms set that I came in with. He suggested cholecystectomy as on option but didn't oversell it: "You may find that those symptoms are not changed after the surgery." He did tell me about a patient who did not want the surgery and managed her illness by diet alone for 20 years until the laparoscopic approach was widely applied. I thought about it for a few minutes and decided to go with it.
Several factors went into my decision. First, when I was a freshman in college I had severe appendicitis and had a gangrenous appendix excised and a Penrose drain hanging out of my side for a week that drained the residual tarry remnants of my appendix. It was the only time in my life I thought I might be better off dead. Friends visiting me at the time thought I was joking. So my first thought was that I dodged a bullet at age 18 and I did not want to take that chance again. And then there was my medical school experience. One of my first patients on surgery was an 85 year old man with an ED diagnosis of acute cholecystitis. He died postoperatively and at autopsy no cause of death was determined. Could I really afford to try to manage this with diet for the rest of my life and take that kind of chance? The other patient was a 45 year old woman who presented with an acute abdomen. In those days imaging not widely applied and the clinical examination was the key determinant. My surgical attending at the time found out that I was considering psychiatry and went out of the way to berate me. He challenged me to tell him the diagnosis on the patient. He was certain it was acute appendicitis. I went with the odds and said she had acute cholecystitis. The surgical team dissected out a normal appendix and he said: "Looks like the medical student was right" and proceeded to make another incision and remove the gallbladder. I put more weight on the imaging, but from my discussion with the surgeon there is still a fair degree of uncertainty. If I am an old man some day with abdominal pain it might be useful to tell the physician: "I don't have an appendix or a gallbladder".
The surgery itself seemed like a breeze. I had a brief pre-op discussion the surgeon and the anesthesiologist. My basic concerns were that I do not get any antibiotic or anesthetic agent that interacts with my existing medication - especially the one that affects cardiac conduction. They assured me that would not be a problem. I also told the anesthesiologist that I had never been intubated before or had either inhaled anesthetics or neuromuscular blockade. I had several minor surgeries where the agents used were fentanyl and midazolam and that seemed to work fine. I also let him know that I have significant arthritis in the neck and he checked it for range of motion. They gave me the intravenous pre-anesthetic, wheeled me into the OR and I was out before I could remember anything. Totally unconscious without a single dream.
In the recovery area I was stoned for about 2 hours. I remember the anesthesiologist coming in and asking me if I had shoulder pain. Sure enough I had significant right should pain. Two milligrams of Dilaudid (hydromorphone) not only cleared that up but it never returned. I had both fentanyl and hydromorphone on board and the nurse kept telling me to "take deep breaths" because my oxygen saturations were dropping. Eventually I woke up completely, got out of bed and started walking around. I reflected back on my gangrenous appendix experience. The day after that surgery, it took two nurses to stand me up next to the bed and then my doctor pushed my chest in order to straighten me up. Every step resulted in severe abdominal pain. That was not the case with this surgery. I had 4 puncture wounds in my abdominal wall but I did not have peritonitis. I was not only moving with ease, but I could also flex my abdominal muscles to get out of bed. The discharge pain medication was oxycodone 5 my every 4 hours as needed. I get headaches and mild nausea from it and stopped it and switched to acetaminophen.
Post op day #1 - I felt progressively worse getting home. It was a general flu-like syndrome with mild nausea. Whenever I think about flu-like illness I think cytokines. There has been a lot of work on that specifically to cholecystectomies, but none of it seems very specific. I was also having difficulty voiding despite having to void as a requirement before being discharged form the hospital. I did not figure out until the next morning that it was probably all part of the physiological changes that occur with abdominal surgery. That was the most interesting chapter in my undergrad surgery text. I also wondered about the oxycodone and the intraoperative cephalospoin (cefazolin) given for antibacterial prophylaxis. I prefer cephalosporins if I need antibiotics and had a similar reaction to cephalexin. With that flu like syndrome I started to get mild tachycardia and blood pressure elevation that I attributed to anxiety about the flu-like symptoms and continued problems with voiding. All of my temps were normal. A final significant symptom was episodes of hiccups, a result of the pneumoperitineum induced to perform abdominal surgery.
Post op day #2 - The voiding problem cleared entirely. Negligible pain. I was contacted in follow up by the hospital. They seemed impressed with the lack of pain, especially shoulder pain and advised me to get active and eat foods that might be beneficial for constipation. The flu-like syndrome seems to be nearly resolved with the exception of some mild facial flushing. At this point it seems like I am on the way to recovery but will post if anything of further interest develops.
I post this experience here to highlight how individual conscious states impact medical decision making - even surgical procedures. I encountered a number of physicians in this process but the core physicians were my primary care internist, the physician who did the pre-op assessment, and the surgeon. My primary care internist has known me for about 30 years. He was highly recommend to me by a psychiatrist who worked in the same clinic. He performed the most thorough examination and gave me a good differential diagnosis at the beginning before ordering the ultrasound. He knows me well enough to know that I am neurotic but when I have a problem it is usually significant. I know that I can expect a very thoughtful analysis of the problems and that he will always call me in follow up. The physician doing the pre-op physical has done two other pre-op physicals on me in the past year. He is also thorough and notes my concerns on the pre-op history and physical - but the problem is that nobody seems to read them after that. The surgeon in this case did a cursory exam but provided me with key information about what to expect up to and including no resolution of symptoms. He didn't have to tell me about the bad outcomes - I was almost a bad outcome myself.
Everything I read about evidence-based medicine and the corporate standardization of medicine minimizes all of the subjective elements that I have listed above. I could have seen different physicians at any step in the above sequence and the outcome may have been different. I could have been a guy who did not have a near death experience with acute appendicitis. I could have had an internist who told me to try simethicone for gas and not ordered the ultrasound. There is also a level of uncertainty that a lot of people seem unaware of. It is certainly possible that I would die from something else and the gallbladder finding was totally incidental. UpToDate suggests that is the course for most incidental gallstones 15-25% become symptomatic in 10-15 years of follow up (1). But was I already symptomatic? Whether it works for the presenting symptoms is still undecided.
There is always room for personal experience and subjectivity in medicine - on the side of the patient as well as the side of the physician. People often refer to this as the art of medicine. The only parallel with art is the apparent creativity that occurs when unique conscious states are all focused on trying to solve the same problem. Informed consent is often seen as a medico-legal procedure but it also acknowledges the subjective experience of both people in the room. There are probably multiple paths to address a problem - but the usual debates I see suggest that there is only one right one.
George Dawson, MD, DFAPA
Reference:
1. Salam F Zakko, MD Section Editor:Sanjiv Chopra, MD Deputy Editor: Shilpa Grover, MD. Overview of gallstone disease in adults. UpToDate. Accessed on January 29, 2019.
Graphics Credit:
The above graphic was downloaded from Shutterstock per their standard agreement.
Sunday, January 27, 2019
Additional Work On The Review of Systems for Psychiatrists
My post on the review of systems for psychiatrists has had a lot of activity lately so I decided to post my latest update that was designed more for patient completion. The original goal was to have an ROS that can be used like the one you get when you check in at an internist or surgeon's office. In the last few years I have been checked in at various offices internists, orthopedic surgeons, ophthalmologists, urologist, and general surgeons) and have seen all of their ROS. All are relatively brief with just a few symptoms.
The historical context of the ROS is that is seems to have migrated from the physicians exam to the waiting room. To a skeptic like me the driving force has been the invasion of real medical services by the business and managed care world. That means much less time with a physician but at the same time requires the physician to document much more. A key piece of that documentation is whether a ROS has been done. Doing the ROS allows for the physician to bill for a more comprehensive assessment. Having a completed form by the patient allows incorporation into the note from that day which has essentially become a billing document. I think it is safe to say that has become the primary role of the ROS today. You will still see your physician for 10 or 15 minutes and they will still ask you some ROS style questions, but they will probably not ask you about the symptoms you endorsed is the waiting room. At least they never asked me.
The traditional role of the ROS is best described in my copy of DeGowin and DeGowin under System Review (Inventory of Systems) (p. 24):
"This is an outline for careful review of the history by inquiring for salient symptoms associated with each system or anatomic region. Primarily it is a search from symptoms that may have escaped the taking of the present illness. These symptoms should be memorized and their diagnostic significance learned. In practice, the answers are not written down except when positive. After the physician has fully mastered the outline, we suggest that he ask the questions when he is examining the part of the body to which they pertain...".
This is followed by a list of 20 symptom headline by body symptoms including the Mental status exam as item number 20.
Over the years there have been additional modifications for psychiatric specialists. The item in DeGowin and DeGowin for the mental status exam is really inadequate for psychiatric purposes so that is expanded and is on its own.
There was some ambivalence by members of the profession about the degree of medical care and knowledge required at one point in time. This led to an expanded "psychiatric review of systems" in some circles that basically looked at symptoms of all of the major categories of psychiatric disorders and counted that as the ROS. It was modified in some standard interviews that were even used in research.
My methods have always been medically based. That was what my mentors taught me and the patients I have treated over the years required. Psychiatrists can never lose sight of the fact that every medication they prescribe has the potential for causing serious medical illness and side effects. We can also never lose sight of the fact that practically every FDA package insert for a medication has medical contraindications, medical complications, and significant medication x underlying illness interactions. Finally, we deal with common medical morbidities like hypertension with severe long term side effects that cannot be ignored.
It is in that spirit that I offer the ROS. It covers what I discuss with patients. It has evolved past the original System Review in that it is now common for patients to have detailed information about relevant diagnoses and diagnostic testing that they have had that is directly relevant to their psychiatric care. I have included that where relevant.
I consider this document to be a starting point. It is for educational purposes only and I am not recommending that you hand it to all of the patients in your waiting area. I suggest modifying it for your needs and rewriting for your specific patient populations. A downloadable Word version is available at the link below. I dictate all of my evaluations and this form also facilitates that process if you need to list specific systems and positive and negative symptoms. As an example, if the ROS is completely negative I will list the first three symptoms as negatives in my dictation.
Any feed back from medical psychiatrists would be greatly appreciated.
George Dawson, MD, DFAPA
References:
1: DeGowin EL, DeGowin RL. Bedside Diagnostic Examination. 3rd Edition. Macmillan Publishing Co. Inc; New York; 1976: p: 24-26.
Review of Systems As A Word Document
Saturday, January 12, 2019
ECT Final Rule
My position on electroconvulsive therapy (ECT) is that it is a safe and effective treatment for severe depression, bipolar depression, and catatonia. It is typically used in the case of treatment resistant depression or acute life threatening psychiatric disorders. In the era prior to modern psychiatric treatment food and water refusal, extreme agitation, uncontrolled aggression, and suicidal behavior all occurred and resulted in excessive mortality. In the case of delirious mania or catatonia, the mortality was estimated as high as 80%. In the acute care setting where I worked we established a practice where two or three physicians in the group specialized in ECT. That was done initially because the malpractice premiums were higher for ECT practitioners. As time went by we were told that from and insurance perspective the risk associated with ECT was not any greater than standard psychiatric practice and all of the premiums became the same. From an actuarial standpoint ECT was considered a safe procedure.
From a cultural perspective ECT is a highly stigmatized treatment. There are very few movies where the public gets a realistic perspective of how it is used. More typically it is presented as a punishment or torture rather than a safe and effective medical treatment. The people I see in consultation are surprised to hear that it is still in use.
Sometime in about 2011, the Food and Drug Administration (FDA) decided to start an initiative about reclassifying ECT devices from Class III (high risk) to Class II (low risk) medical devices. In their classification system for medical devices, Class III is the most restrictive because it requires premarket approval. Class II is less restrictive because it can be approved with special controls or recommended measures to mitigate risk. Class I is least restrictive and requires general controls. The reason why the FDA initiated this reclassification attempt in 2011 is unclear to me. The reason why it initially failed is fairly common knowledge. The antipsychiatry movement has been against ECT since before the time of Breggin's protest (1) in the New England Journal of Medicine over a book review by Mandel (2) that discredited his negative assessment of ECT. A quote from Mandel's review from 40 years ago:
"Dr. Breggin's arguments fail because he uses supporting data uncritically and inaccurately. At a time when reasoned discourse and scientific exchange concerning ECT are needed, he simply calls for the abolition of the treatment on the basis of his personal conclusions. A critical reader will find this book of interest only as an example of how the fires of controversy can be fanned by emotion."
Any time I have attempted to debate the merits of ECT in a public forum (like Twitter) there is a generally trend among the antipsychiatrists to jump on whatever I say and with quote Breggin's book or post links to his dated and inaccurate work. Irrespective of how the FDA initiative started it did offer some hope that a neutral federal agency could put some of this controversy to rest.
The FDA came out with the Final order on the reclassification of ECT devices on December 26, 2018. As far as I can tell there was no fanfare. Deflating controversy typically has that effect. It took me about three hours to read through the document and it was a very interesting read. The FDA used various sources to look at the issues of what they abbreviate as SE or safety and effectiveness. They go though their decision making systematically including a section at the end where they address criticisms of ECT. the FDA, and in some cases professional organizations that suggest ECT is safe and effective like the American Psychiatric Association (APA). The FDA gives an unequivocal response to these questions "The FDA disagrees with ......."
There is a very interesting section on the most controversial aspect of ECT - memory loss. I have excerpted the studies and FDA summary statements in the table below:
The FDA considered over 400 scientific papers for this reclassification of ECT. The examples of their conclusory statements about memory related problems in the table above is consistent with clinical practice. They are careful to point out that there are reports of some people who state they have had some permanent memory loss but they do not get into the potential explanations for that phenomenon. They emphasize that the FDA's role is to comment on safety and efficacy and not purported neurobiological mechanisms.
There is an extensive comments section where the FDA summarizes arguments from hundreds of comments and answers them definitively. Many of these comments are right out of the antipsychiatry playbook. Consider the comment below from page page 66113. This is a direct excerpt from the Federal Register:
(Comment 8) Several comments indicated that ECT should be banned. Several comments characterized ECT as inhumane. Commenters indicated that the United Nations Special Rapporteur on Torture and Other Cruel Inhuman or Degrading Treatment or Punishment February 16, 2013, defined ECT without consent as torture.
(Response 8) FDA disagrees that ECT should be banned. Section 516 of the FD&C Act (21 U.S.C. 360f) authorizes FDA to ban a device when, based on all available data and information, FDA finds that the device ‘‘presents substantial deception or an unreasonable and substantial risk of illness or injury.’’ During review of the scientific evidence, FDA did not identify sufficient evidence to ban ECT. FDA determined that special controls, in combination with general controls, can mitigate the identified risks of ECT for certain intended uses and mitigate risks associated with ECT use. FDA determined that there is a reasonable assurance of SE for ECT treatment for the identified indications for use and patient populations. Therefore, FDA has determined that ECT does not present substantial deception or an unreasonable and substantial risk of illness or injury
The FDA in its disagreement with most of these negative comments - is in line with psychiatric practice and the obvious facts that if ECT was ineffective or resulted in significant injury - psychiatrists would not be using it or doing more extensive research on similar neurostimulation techniques to make neuromodulation as noninvasive and effective as possible.
All things considered this was a very positive statement about ECT. The FDA points out the limitations of their regulatory scope. For example, even though they did not include an extensive list of conditions as indications for ECT - they acknowledge that once a device is approved it can be used for off-label conditions. They are also careful to point out that they are not endorsing ECT as the treatment of choice for the named conditions and it is never used that way. Their regulatory language specifies "treatment resistant" and "require rapid response" as specifying the clinical population for which ECT benefits out weigh the risks.
I don't see psychiatrists having any problem with that language since that is the population we have always used this modality for.
George Dawson, MD, DFAPA
References:
1: Breggin PR. Electroconvulsive therapy for depression. N Engl J Med. 1980 Nov27;303(22):1305-6. PubMed PMID: 7421975.
2: Mandel MR. Electroshock: Its brain-disabling effects (book review). August 14, 1980
N Engl J Med 1980; 303:402 DOI: 10.1056/NEJM198008143030721.
3: Food and Drug Administration, HHS. Neurological Devices; Reclassification of Electroconvulsive Therapy Devices; Effective Date of Requirement for Premarket Approval for Electroconvulsive Therapy Devices for Certain Specified Intended Uses. Final order. Fed Regist. 2018 Dec 26;83(246):66103-24. PubMed PMID:30596410.
.
Supplementary:
Brandolini's Law is well illustrated by the tactics used by antipsychiatrists in any public forum. In an ideal world the FDA document would be the definitive word on ECT. I am not that optimistic but encourage people to bookmark this place in the Federal Register and refer to it if you see heated debates about ECT, especially where it is being portrayed as being toxic and/or ineffective.
The FDA came out with the Final order on the reclassification of ECT devices on December 26, 2018. As far as I can tell there was no fanfare. Deflating controversy typically has that effect. It took me about three hours to read through the document and it was a very interesting read. The FDA used various sources to look at the issues of what they abbreviate as SE or safety and effectiveness. They go though their decision making systematically including a section at the end where they address criticisms of ECT. the FDA, and in some cases professional organizations that suggest ECT is safe and effective like the American Psychiatric Association (APA). The FDA gives an unequivocal response to these questions "The FDA disagrees with ......."
There is a very interesting section on the most controversial aspect of ECT - memory loss. I have excerpted the studies and FDA summary statements in the table below:
Reference
|
FDA
Comments (excerpted)
|
Fernie,
G., et al., ‘‘Detecting Objective and Subjective Cognitive Effects of
Electroconvulsive Therapy: Intensity, Duration and Test Utility in a Large
Clinical Sample.’’ Psychological Medicine, 2014. 44(14): pp. 2985–2994.
|
Overall,
the application of ECT had reversible cognitive deficiencies compared to
preECT treatment scores, a measure of safety, and in some assessments
(CANTAB, subjective reports of memory function, and MMSE) showed patient
improvement.
|
Kirov,
G.G., et al., ‘‘Evaluation of Cumulative Cognitive Deficits from
Electroconvulsive Therapy.’’ British Journal of Psychiatry, 2016. 208(3): pp.
266–270.
|
Not all
subjects were capable of performing all tests and parts of the battery
changed over time. Results (linear mixed regression analyses) demonstrated
that age, severity of depression at the time of testing, and number of days
since the last ECT session were the major factors affecting cognitive
performance, but the total number of previous ECT sessions did not have a
measurable impact on cognitive performance, which further supports the safety
of ECT in not leading to cumulative cognitive deficits.
|
Maric,
N.P., et al., ‘‘The Acute and Medium-Term Effects of Treatment with
Electroconvulsive Therapy on Memory in Patients with Major Depressive
Disorder.’’ Psychological Medicine, 2016. 46(4): pp. 797–806.
|
At the
same time, the neuropsychological tests did not detect any significant memory
impairment and showed improvement on visual memory and learning at 1 month
and in the immediate post-treatment period, indicating no prolonged or
significant ECT-related memory deficits. These improvements correlated with
improvement in depression while serious adverse events were not reported.
|
Spaans,
H.P., et al., ‘‘Efficacy and Cognitive Side Effects After Brief Pulse and
Ultrabrief Pulse Right Unilateral Electroconvulsive Therapy for Major
Depression: A Randomized, DoubleBlind, Controlled Study.’’ Journal of
Clinical Psychiatry, 2013. 74(11): pp. e1029–1036.
|
No
significant difference was seen in retrograde amnesia between the two
treatment groups. Change in recall performance and fluency tests were also
similar between the two groups. There was not a significant difference in
performance in the cognitive tests following ECT for any of the cognitive
tests during the course of study. The authors also reported mitigating
adverse effects on cognition by lengthening the time between treatments to
provide patients with more time to recuperate, thereby further characterizing
how ECT treatment can be applied safely.
|
Ghaziuddin,
N., et al., ‘‘Cognitive Side Effects of Electroconvulsive Therapy in
Adolescents.’’ Journal of Child Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 2000. 10(4):
pp. 269–276
|
The
comparison of pre-ECT and the immediate post-ECT testing demonstrated
significant impairments of concentration and attention, verbal and
visual-delayed recall, and verbal fluency. A complete recovery of these
functions was noted in the cognitive testing conducted at 8.5 months. There
was no deficit in the ability to problem solve during the initial or the
subsequent testing. Cognitive parameters found to be impaired during the
first few days of ECT were recovered over several months following the
treatment. Therefore, there was no evidence of long-term damage to
concentration, attention, verbal and visual memory, or verbal fluency. There
were also no impairments of motor strength and executive processing, even
during the early (within 7 to 10 days) post-ECT period.
|
The FDA considered over 400 scientific papers for this reclassification of ECT. The examples of their conclusory statements about memory related problems in the table above is consistent with clinical practice. They are careful to point out that there are reports of some people who state they have had some permanent memory loss but they do not get into the potential explanations for that phenomenon. They emphasize that the FDA's role is to comment on safety and efficacy and not purported neurobiological mechanisms.
There is an extensive comments section where the FDA summarizes arguments from hundreds of comments and answers them definitively. Many of these comments are right out of the antipsychiatry playbook. Consider the comment below from page page 66113. This is a direct excerpt from the Federal Register:
(Comment 8) Several comments indicated that ECT should be banned. Several comments characterized ECT as inhumane. Commenters indicated that the United Nations Special Rapporteur on Torture and Other Cruel Inhuman or Degrading Treatment or Punishment February 16, 2013, defined ECT without consent as torture.
(Response 8) FDA disagrees that ECT should be banned. Section 516 of the FD&C Act (21 U.S.C. 360f) authorizes FDA to ban a device when, based on all available data and information, FDA finds that the device ‘‘presents substantial deception or an unreasonable and substantial risk of illness or injury.’’ During review of the scientific evidence, FDA did not identify sufficient evidence to ban ECT. FDA determined that special controls, in combination with general controls, can mitigate the identified risks of ECT for certain intended uses and mitigate risks associated with ECT use. FDA determined that there is a reasonable assurance of SE for ECT treatment for the identified indications for use and patient populations. Therefore, FDA has determined that ECT does not present substantial deception or an unreasonable and substantial risk of illness or injury
The FDA in its disagreement with most of these negative comments - is in line with psychiatric practice and the obvious facts that if ECT was ineffective or resulted in significant injury - psychiatrists would not be using it or doing more extensive research on similar neurostimulation techniques to make neuromodulation as noninvasive and effective as possible.
All things considered this was a very positive statement about ECT. The FDA points out the limitations of their regulatory scope. For example, even though they did not include an extensive list of conditions as indications for ECT - they acknowledge that once a device is approved it can be used for off-label conditions. They are also careful to point out that they are not endorsing ECT as the treatment of choice for the named conditions and it is never used that way. Their regulatory language specifies "treatment resistant" and "require rapid response" as specifying the clinical population for which ECT benefits out weigh the risks.
I don't see psychiatrists having any problem with that language since that is the population we have always used this modality for.
George Dawson, MD, DFAPA
References:
1: Breggin PR. Electroconvulsive therapy for depression. N Engl J Med. 1980 Nov27;303(22):1305-6. PubMed PMID: 7421975.
2: Mandel MR. Electroshock: Its brain-disabling effects (book review). August 14, 1980
N Engl J Med 1980; 303:402 DOI: 10.1056/NEJM198008143030721.
3: Food and Drug Administration, HHS. Neurological Devices; Reclassification of Electroconvulsive Therapy Devices; Effective Date of Requirement for Premarket Approval for Electroconvulsive Therapy Devices for Certain Specified Intended Uses. Final order. Fed Regist. 2018 Dec 26;83(246):66103-24. PubMed PMID:30596410.
Supplementary:
Brandolini's Law is well illustrated by the tactics used by antipsychiatrists in any public forum. In an ideal world the FDA document would be the definitive word on ECT. I am not that optimistic but encourage people to bookmark this place in the Federal Register and refer to it if you see heated debates about ECT, especially where it is being portrayed as being toxic and/or ineffective.
Subscribe to:
Posts (Atom)